Document Type : Research Article

Authors

Urmia

Abstract

Introduction
Adaptive management of emotions is essential for mental health (Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema, and Schweizer, 2010; Eisenberg, Fabes, Guthrie, and Reiser, 2000). The emotional regulation involves monitoring, evaluating, and modifying emotional responses to achieve goals (Thompson, 1994). There are many individual differences in emotional abilities, including the identification and understanding of their emotions and others, the causes and effects of various emotions and the strategies used to regulate emotions (Cummings, 1987; Denham, 1998). These differences in emotional processes and management of emotions have wide implications in many psychological functions (Denham, 1986; Eisenberg et al., 2000). Findings show that emotional dysregulation affects the psychiatric pathology (Aldao et al., 2010). Among the pathological traits that have attracted a lot of attention in recent years, are dark traits. Dark traits are Narcissistic, Machiavellianism and Psychopathic, which are independent, but related to one another (Palos and Williams, 2002). The defect in the emotional experience is considered as the main factor in the dark traits (Jansson and Kross, 2013). The biological structure of the personality that appears to be involved in explaining individual differences in personality dark tendencies is the morning-evening type. The trait of morning-evening is known as the biological dimension of the personality, which is largely the result of heredity (Adan, Archer, Hidalgo, Di Milia, Natale, and Randler, 2012). In addition to the physiological differences between morning to evening type (for example, patterns of cortisol and melatonin secretion), some psychological differences have also been reported between these two types. The current study investigated the relationships between morningness-eveningness, emotion and emotion regulation beliefsand dark traits.
 
Method
The study participants were 439 students who were selected through multi-stage cluster sampling. Inclusion criteria were being aged 18 or older and all participants reported that they had not been diagnosed as a medical case (physical, but not necessarily psychological disorders) by a doctor. Participants with a psychotic disorder, abuse acute suicidality, insufficient language skills, or severe cognitive impairment were excluded. Participants completed a series of questionnaires following an informed consent procedure and were provided with debriefing information on the purpose of the study and given a list of community counseling agencies at the end of the survey. Of the total participants, 404 were undergraduate student (92%), and 35 ones were masters and higher education students (8%), 139 participants were male (31.7%) and 300 ones were female (68.3%). Respectively, the students' ages' mean and standard deviation were 19.85 and 1.78 in males, 19.81 and 1.81 in females.
 
Results
Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations among the study variables were presented in Table 1. Skew ness and kurtosis were within a conventional range (−1 to 1; George and Mallery, 2003) for all of the variables. Bivariate correlations showed that regulation worth, as a positive emotional attitude, was associated with morningness tendencies(see Table 1). Morningness positively associated with narcissism and psychopathic tendencies (see Table 1). The negative dimensions of emotional attitudes, including emotion constraint and hijacking were positively associated with dark triad traits, but the regulation worth, as a positive emotional attitude had negative correlations with Machiavellianism and Narcissism and a positive correlation with psychopathic tendencies. Moderation was tested using Sobel’s test. We tested whether the emotional attitudes moderated the relationships between the Morningness-Eveningness and dark triad traits. In order to determine the extent to which emotional attitudes were involved in these moderation effects, we conducted the moderation analyses for dark triad traits. The difference in narcissism trait was partially moderated by regulation worth (z=2.01, p<.05). The difference in psychopathy trait was partially moderated by regulation worth (z=2.34, p<.05). In other cases, dimensions of emotional attitudes were not moderating the path between morningness-eveningness and dark triad traits (See table 3).
 
Discussion and Conclusion
The aim of the current study was to examine whether emotion and emotion regulation beliefscan help explain the relationship between morningness-eveningness and dark traits. As was expected, negative emotion beliefs (Emotion Constraint and Hijacking) were associated with dark traits. In addition, results from the moderator analysis suggest that the relationship between morningness-eveningness and dark traits were partially mediated via emotion and emotion regulation beliefs. We believe that these findings add knowledge to the emotion and emotion beliefs and demonstrate the importance of emotion beliefs and emotion dysregulation to psychopathology in various ways. Despite interesting findings, there are several limitations of the current study which worth noting. First, participants’ report was obtained retrospectively, therefore, recall bias could have impacted some of the participants’ self-reporting. Additionally, it is unclear whether the sample of the present study is presenting with diagnosable levels of the dark traits symptoms.

Keywords

Adan, A, Archer, S. N, Hidalgo, M. P, Di Milia, L, Natale, V, & Randler, C. (2012). Circadian typology: A comprehensive review, Chronobiology International, 29, 1153–1175.
Aftab, R., Taghilo, S., & Migoni, M. (2017). The structural model of relationship between borderline personality traits, interpersonal difficulties and Internet addiction. Achievements (Journal of Education & Psychology), 23 (2), 113-136. [Persian].
Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Mechanisms, 30 (2), 217-37.
Ali, F., Amorim, I. S., & Chamorro-Premuzic, T. (2009). Empathy deficits and trait emotional intelligence in psychopathy and Machiavellianism. Personality andIndividual Differences, 47, 758-762.
Amiri, S., & Mikaeli Manee, F. (2016). Evaluation of state-trait anxiety and Difficulties in Emotion Regulation: Based on personality dimensions of sensitivity to punishment and sensitivity to reward. Journal of Psychological Achievements (Journal of Education & Psychology), 4, 1 (15); 115 - 136. [Persian].
Amiri, S., & Yaghobi, A. (2016). Evaluation of the psychometric properties scale of Dark Triad of personality. Research in Clinical Psychology and Counseling’s, 6, 1. [Persian].
Atashafrouz , A., & Araban, A. (2017). The Causal Relationship between Personality Traits and Academic Performance with Mediating Role of Study Strategies in Students. Psychological Achievements (Journal of Education & Psychology), 24 (1), 78-98. [Persian].
Austin, E. J., Farrelly, D., Black, C., & Moore, H. (2007). Emotional intelligence, Machiavellianism and emotional manipulation: Does EI have a dark side? Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 179-189.
Bardeen, J.  R., Kumpula, M. J., & Orcutt, H. K. (2013).  Emotion regulation difficulties as a prospective predictor of posttraumatic stress symptoms following a mass shooting. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 27, 188–196. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2013.01.003.
Barnow, S., Aldinger, M., Ulrich, I., & Stopsack, M. (2013). Emotionsregulation bei Depression: Ein multimethodaler Überblick. Psychologische Rundschau, 64, 235–243.
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173.
Bohnert, A. M., Crnic, K. A., & Lim, K. G. (2003). Emotional competence and aggressive behavior in school-age children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31,79-91.
Brook, M., & Kosson, D. S. (2013). Impaired cognitive empathy in criminal psychopathy: Evidence from a laboratory measure of empathic accuracy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122, 156–166.
Buss, D. M. (2009). How can evolutionary psychology explain personality and individual differences? Perspectives in Psychological Science, 4, 359–366.Adan, A., Archer, SN., Hidalgo, MP., Di Milia, L., Natale, V., Randler, C. Circadian typology: A comprehensive review. Chronobiology International, 29 (9), 1153-1175.
Caci, H., Robert, P., & Boyer, P. (2004). Novelty seekers and impulsive subjects are low in morningness. European Psychiatry, 19, 79–84.
Campos, J. J., Frankel, C. B., & Camras, L. A. (2004). On the nature of emotion regulation. Child Development, 75, 377e394.
Cloitre, M., Koenen, K. C., Cohen, L. R., & Han, H. (2002). Skills training in affective and interpersonal regulation followed by exposure: A phase-based treatment for PTSD related to childhood abuse. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 1067–1074.
Cole, P. M., Martin, S. E., & Dennis, T. A. (2004). Emotion regulation as a scientific construct: Methodological challenges and directions for child development research. Child Development, 75, 317e333.
Cummings, E. M. (1987). Coping with background anger in early childhood. Child Development, 58, 976e984.
Denham, S. A. (1986). Social cognition, prosocial behavior, and emotion in preschoolers: contextual validation. Child Development, 57, 194e201.
Denham, S. A. (1998). Emotional development in young children. New York: Guilford Press.
Díaz-Morales, J. F. (2007). Morning and evening-types: Exploring their personality styles. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 769–778.
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Guthrie, I. K., & Reiser, M. (2000). Dispositional emotionality and regulation: Their role in predicting quality of social functioning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 136e157.
George D., Mallery P. SPSS for Windows step by step:  a simple guide and reference. 11. 0 update (4th ed.).  Boston, MA:  Allyn & Bacon; 2003.
George, D., & Mallery, P. (2003). SPSS for Windows step by step: A simple guide and reference. 11. 0 update (4th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Goldsmith, R. E., Chesney, S. A., Heath, N. M., & Barlow, M. R. (2013).  Emotion  regulation difficulties mediate associations between betrayal trauma and symptoms of posttraumatic stress, depression, and anxiety. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 26, 376–384.
Gross, J. J., & Muñoz, R. F. (1995). Emotion regulation and mental health. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 2, 151e164.
Horn, J. A., Ostenberg, O. A. (1976). Self-Assesment Questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. International Journal of Chronobiology, 74, 97-110.
Jakobwitz, S., & Egan, V. (2006). The Dark Triad and normal personality traits.Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 331-339.
Johnson, A. M., Vernon, P. A., & Feiler, A. R. (2008). Behavioral genetic studies of personality: An introduction and review of the results of 50+ years of research. In G. J. Boyle, G. Matthews, & D. H. Saklofske (Eds.), The Sage handbook of personality theory and assessment, Volume 1.Los Angeles: Sage.
Jonason, P. K., & Krause, L. (2013). The emotional deficits associated with the Dark Triad traits: Cognitive empathy, affective empathy, and alexithymia. Personalityand Individual Differences, 55, 532-537.
Jonason, P. K., Webster, G. D., Schmitt, D. P., Li, N. P., & Crysel, L. (2012). The antihero in popular culture: A life history theory of the Dark Triad. Review of General Psychology, 16, 192–199.
Jones, D. N., & Paulhus, D. L. (2014). Introducing the Short Dark Triad (SSD3): A brief measure of dark personality trait. Assessment, 21, 28-41.
Killgore, W. D. S. (2007). Effects of sleep-deprivation and morningness–eveningness traits on risk-taking. Psychological Reports, 100, 613–626.
Kowalski, R. M. (Ed.). (2001). Behaving badly: Aversive behaviors in interpersonal relationships. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Lee, K., & Ashton, M. C. (2005). Psychopathy, Machiavellianism, and narcissism in the Five-Factor Model and the HEXACO model of personality structure. Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 1571-1582.
Levenson, M. R., Kiehl, K. A., & Fitzpatrick, C. M. (1995). Assessing psychopathic attributes in a noninstitutionalised population. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 151–158.
Maestripieri, D. (2014). Night owl women are similar to men in their relationship orientation, risk-taking propensities, and cortisol levels: Implications for the adaptive significance and evolution of eveningness. Evolutionary Psychology, 12, 130–147.
Malterer, M. B., Glass, S. J., & Newman, J. P. (2008). Psychopathy and trait emotional intelligence. Personalityand Individual Differences, 44, 735-745.
Matthews, G. (1988). Morningness–eveningness as a dimension of personality: Trait, state and psychophysiological correlates. European Journal of Personality, 2, 277–293.
Moulton, S. J., Newman, E., Power, K., Swanson, V., & Day, K. (2015). Childhood trauma and eating psychopathology: A mediating role for dissociation and emotion dysregulation? Child Abuse & Neglect, 39, 167–174.
Patrick, C. J., & Lang, A. R. (1999). Psychopathic traits andintoxicated states: Affective concomitants and conceptuallinks. In M. E. Dawson, A. M. Schell, & A. H. Boehmelt (Eds.), Startle modification: Implications for neuroscience, cognitive science, and clinical science. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Paulhus, D. L., & Jones, D. N. (2011). A short measure of the Dark Triad. Presented at meeting of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, San Antonio, Texas.
Paulhus, D. L., & Williams, K. M. (2002). The Dark Triad of personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy. Journal of Research in Personality, 36, 556–563.
Rahafar, A., Sadeghi Jojeili, M., Sadeghpour, A., & Mirzaie, S., (2013). Surveying psychometric features of Persian version of morning-eventide questionnaire. Clinical Psychology & Personality. 20 (8), 109-122. [Persian].
Randler, C. (2007a). Gender differences in morningness–eveningness assessed by self-report questionnaires: A metaanalysis, Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 1667-1675.
Randler, C. (2008). Morningness-eveningness, sleep-wake variables and big-five personality factors, Personality and Individual Differences, 45, 191-96.
Sabouri, S., Gerber, M., Sadeghi Bahmani, D., Lemola, S., Clough, PJ., Kalak, N., Shamsi, M., Holsboer-Trachsler, E., Brand, S. (2016). Examining dark triad traits in relation to mental toughness and physical activity in young adults. Neuropsychiatric disease and treatment, 12, 229-235.
Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Aharon-Peretz, J., & Perry, D. (2009). Two systems for empathy: A double dissociation between emotional and cognitive empathy in inferior frontal gyrus versus ventromedial prefrontal lesions. Brain, 12, 617–627.
Suveg, C., & Zeman, J. (2004). Emotion regulation in children with anxiety disorders. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33, 750-759.
Thompson, R. A. (1994). Emotion regulation: A theme in search of definition. Monographs for the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, 25-52.
Twenge, J. M., & Campbell, K. W. (2003). ‘‘Isn’t it fun to get the respect that we’re going to deserve?’’ Narcissism, social rejection, and aggression. Personality andSocial Psychology Bulletin, 29, 261–272.
Veilleux, J. C., Salomaa, A. C., Shaver, J. A., Zielinski, M. J., & Pollert, G. A. (2015). Multidimensional Assessment of Beliefs About Emotion: Development and Validation of the Emotion and Regulation Beliefs Scale. Assessment, 22 (1), 86-100.
Wai, M., & Tiliopoulos, N. (2012). The affective and cognitive empathic nature of the dark triad of personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 52, 794-799.